Model Imperative

Share This
« Back to Glossary Index

Joseph Chilton Pearce shares the model imperative in the essay below (from Touch the Future’s Joseph Chilton Pearce Library and Academy):

If we understood the true nature that the capacity to imagin represents we would treat every child, our approach to education and society profoundly differently.

The dictionary defines imagination as the ability to create images not present to the sensory system.  If we knew the critical nature of imagination, how it’s the very core, the very foundation of all higher stages of learning and intellectual life, we would certainly look at the child differently.

First, play is the major intelligence of the first seven years and even the first 11 years of life. Again, we’ll put up our birth, age one, age four, seven and 11, and we will go on and put 15 for now, as our major developmental stages, regardless of whether you agree with all the Piaget content of those stages, we do know those stages are biologically unfolding at those times.

From the seventh month in utero, before a child is born, every time the mother uses a word, each phenome name of the word, and we are talking about vibrations, a word is just a vibration of sound, each vibration, called a phenome brings a muscular response in the infant.

From the very beginning, there is this intimate connection between body, body movement, the brain, and its formation of word structures. By the time the child is born, this is myelinated, it’s locked in as a permanent structure.

In what we call the in arms period, the eyes, the visual process dominates. Things aren’t auditory as much as they are visual in those first few months. Why? Because we couldn’t develop vision in utero, and so the first few months, everything is visual to that child.  They are looking, looking, looking, absorbing enormous amounts about their visual world.  Around six to 12 months, we have what Piaget called object constancy. The child’s visual world, simply, suddenly stabilizes.  We know this is brought about by myelination of the axons involved in all sorts of other maturation processes. The auditory fields and the visual fields of the brain stabilize, they mature enough so total entrainment on the visual process is no longer needed somewhere around the first year of life. And language and walking appear.

Now, let’s look at the growth of language itself, and the relationship between word and thing. I love the work of Blurton Jones, working with Tenbergen, the Nobel laureate, in the cross cultural study of the pointing syndrome. When the little child is in their own nest, they think anything is safe to interact with. And just watch it, a child in a house, anything, they will just jump right in on it.  They want to taste it, touch it, smell it, feel it, and immediately say, what is that mamma, what is that daddy. They are asking for a name label for the object.

When you give them a name, that word for a thing, the word and the thing build into the brain as a single neural pattern. The brain does not build a neural network of the thing, its taste, touch, smell, feel and in quality, and then, in addition, add to that pattern its name, as though the name were a separate item. The name builds in as an integral part of the whole structure of knowledge, as Piaget calls it.  A structure of knowledge is a neural pattern that results from the child’s inner action with an object or an event of his physical world out there.  And the brain responds by creating those neural fields that can handle that kind of phenomena. The name and the thing build as a single unit.

We call this concrete language. The word doesn’t stand for the thing, the word and the thing are the same to the early child.  Ask the two-year-old child to say the word ‘hand,’ and they move their hand when they say it.  Why?  Because the word ‘hand’ means something very tangible, something very concrete.  They can’t deal with abstractions at all.

When you take the child out in the open, away from the nest, all mammalian animals respond the same way.  The child spots an object, let’s say, it’s a dirty, nasty, old dog. If they have never seen one before, they stop, point toward the object, and silently turn around and stare at the caretaker, whoever it is, grandma, papa, mamma, and wait for some kind of a signal from the parent that they perceive this particular object.  Getting that signal the child interacts with the object, to taste it, touch it, smell it, feel it, talk to it and build, what we call, a sensory-motor structure of knowledge of that object, and of course, immediately want to know, what is that mamma. They ask for a name.

Let’s suppose that it is a dirty, nasty, awful looking, old mongrel dog.  Mamma says, don’t you dare touch that dirty, nasty, mongrel, old dog. Her acknowledgment of the dog is all the child needs. This is the model imperative. The child must perceive some kind of verification that the mother is interacting and perceiving this object. With that verification, the child rushes over to interact with the dirty, nasty, old dog.  Mamma saying don’t, has nothing to do with it.  The child is impelled.  They are driven, by nature, to interact with the object and build a structure of knowledge of it.

Now, the mother’s emotional state of that; her horror, alarm, etc., etc., builds into the structure of knowledge the child creates as an integral part of it. Her name for the object, and her emotional state are built into the structure of knowledge of the object the child creates. All without any evaluation on the part of the child. If it’s a beautiful flower and the mother smiles and the child rushes over to do what, to pull it off its stem, stuff it in his mouth, taste it, touch it, smell it, feel it, and so on, the mother’s emotional state of approval is built into that structure of knowledge along with the word flower.

« Back to Glossary Index

Comments are closed.