Decolonizing Child Raising

Child raising in the western world is off the rails.

Key points:

  • Colonizers planted trauma all over the world.
  • Colonizer-European child raising traumatizes children.
  • Traditional Indigenous child raising provides nested support and fosters wellbeing.

Efforts to decolonize minds and hearts has been ongoing in educational circles for some time (Four Arrows, 2011). Using “decolonization” as a metaphor is controversial (Tuck & Wang, 2012) because it too often ignores its original unsettling meaning of removing colonization from Indigenous lands. However, there is an internal colonization within the human psyche that needs to be addressed. European colonialization has affected nearly all bodies on the planet, but also minds. Bodies and psyches have been starved of their basic needs, minds genocidally cleansed, and sometimes disabled in their cultural diversity (Adams, 2020; Davis, 2007; Narvaez, 2014; de Sousa Santos, 2018).

When we think of what the colonizers did and still do, we think mostly of what they take away or tried to take away—e.g., cultural and spiritual practices, communal sharing, respect for elders, self-respect, and ecological intelligence (Dunbar-Ortiz, 2014; Trafzer, Keller & Sisquoc, 2006). We don’t typically think of the legacy they gave everyone, a legacy that still haunts us all. What settlers and colonizers gave everyone is trauma. Many brought their own inherited intergenerational experiences of trauma from millennia-old European practices of mistreating children, women and the unfortunate (Buck, 2019; deMause, 1995; Greven, 1977, 1991; Scott, 1968). Then with wétiko cruelty (Forbes, 2008), they passed the trauma to all they met outside their homelands, embedding it in bodies and minds. And these bodies and minds, unable to use traditional healing practices, often passed the trauma to the next generations (Menakem, 2017). One of the strongest forms of trauma transmission occurs through child raising, as interdisciplinary research is demonstrating (Garner et al., 2021; Lanius, Pain & Vermetten, 2010). 

Settlers brought their harsh parenting practices to the “new world,” shocking Native Peoples, who would step in to prevent it when the settlers tried to whip children (Greer, 2000). In turn, early settler recollections indicated astonishment that the Native Nations did not punish their children.

Settlers brought a European pattern of unnested care (e.g., isolation apart from the community, corporal punishment, unwelcoming community, nature disconnection). Colonizing child raising practices are flavored with ideas of original sin (Jacobs, 2001)—that babies are born in sin and must be coerced to be good. Therefore, babies’ needs are minimized (‘they are resilient’). Babies were and still are coerced into schedules for eating and sleeping, and even spanked, “for their own good” (Lee, Grogan-Kaylor & Berger, 2014; Miller, 1990). They are spend a great deal of time physically isolated (not in arms) and if they cry it is considered ‘what babies do.’ All these ideas and practices are contrary to humanity’s heritage as documented by ethnographers (Hewlett & Lamb, 2005; Konner, 2005).

Babies have a built-in compass that indicates good or bad feeling. When needed support is not provided, the alarm bell goes off. Babies initially indicate discomfort with a grimace of gesture. Crying is a late signal of distress. But babies are often left to cry in settler child raising. Instead of meeting the needs of babies, babies often are held in contempt, as if they are manipulators of adults, as if meeting their needs will “spoil” them. Adults think they have to win a power game or the child will be a terror. Neuroscience and clinical studies show us that the opposite is the case. Leaving a baby in distress causes toxic stress for developing brain systems (Murgatroyd & Spengler, 2011) and for psychological development (Moloney, 1949; Ribble, 1943; Winnicott, 1987). 

Neuroscientific studies now show us the mechanisms of how trauma affects child brain development in the short and long term. When babies are distressed too long, intensely, or routinely, brain development is thwarted by the overproduction of cortisol, killing brain cells and their connections (McEwen, 2017; Murgatroyd & Spengler, 2011). Unsupportive care enhances survival systems in the brain and self-protectionist social attitudes and behavior, making self-breakdown or oppositionalism more likely to become part of the personality than cooperation (Narvaez, 2014; Schore, 2003).

So if we are going to decolonize ourselves thoroughly, we need to decolonize trauma-inducing child raising practices and return to the wellness-promoting practices of our ancestors (Gleason & Narvaez, 2019). See the chart for a contrast between traditional Indigenous and European-colonizing child raising practices, below. See, and download, the Worldview Chart comparing Dominant and Indigenous Worldview, by Four Arrows, here.

Traditional First Nation and Native Peoples of the world, particularly hunter-gatherers, are known for their “indulgent” (loving) treatment of children, shocking western explorers, settlers, researchers who documented it (Konner, 2005). They provided our species’ evolved nest (Narvaez, 2014, 2019). Like every animal’s nest, humanity’s nest evolved to meet the maturational needs of the child to ensure optimal normal development (Gottlieb, 2002). It includes soothing birth and perinatal experiences (which includes extensive maternal support), breastfeeding on request for several years, responsive care to keep baby comforted and calm, a welcoming community and a set of supportive stable caregivers, extensive affectionate touch (and no negative touch), self-directed social play, nature immersion and attachment, and routine healing practices (Hewlett & Lamb, 2005; Narvaez, 2021; Narvaez & Tarsha, 2021). 

Understanding the neediness of babies is the first step (Narvaez, 2014). Babies resemble fetuses of other animals till at least 18 months of age and so need extensive assistance in growing brain and body well, including learning to breathe, maintain heartrate, digestion, and a calm physiology (Montagu, 1968, 1970). The provision of the nest ensures a well-functioning neurobiology (e.g., stress response), which mostly grows after birth and undergirds flexible intelligence and sociality (Carter & Porges, 2013). 

The second step is understanding the effects of not meeting young children’s basic needs. (Karr-Morse & Wiley, 1997, 2012). Third, to avoid the myth of spoiling babies with too much attention, the third step is to understand that children cannot consciously manipulate adults until perhaps four years of age (they don’t have the cognitive capacity until around that time). The final step is to realize that children whose caregivers meet their needs from the beginning (providing the evolved nest) develop into cooperative members of the community, as can be noted from the personality and social characteristics of nest-providing communities (Narvaez, 2013, 2019).

According to First Nation child raising practices around the world, to raise a good child you let them make their own choices and honor their unique spirit (MacPherson & Rabb, 2011). The community supports them through the evolved nest and surrounds them with stories, role models and daily rituals showing what being a good member of an earth-respecting community looks like (Cajete, 2001). Integrated into community life, children grow into their uniqueness and into goodness when scaffolded with such practices. The evolved nest is designed for this.

Explore our Evolved Nest’s nine components in the online self-directed learning center!

References

Adams, D.W. (2020). Education for extinction: American Indians and the Boarding School Experience, 1875-1928, 2nded. University Press of Kansas.

Buck, P.D. (2019). The punishment monopoly: Tales of my ancestors, dispossession, and the building of the United States. Monthly Review Press.

Cajete, G. (2000). Native science: Natural laws of interdependence. Santa Fe: Clear Light.

Davis, W. (2007). Light at the edge of the world: A journey through the realm of vanishing cultures. Vancouver: Douglas & MacIntyre.

De Sousa Santos, B. (2018). The end of the cognitive empire: The coming of age of epistemologies of the south. Duke University Press.

deMause, L. (1995). The history of childhood. New York, NY: Psychohistory Press.

Dunbar-Ortiz, R. (2014). An indigenous people’s history of the United States. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.

Forbes, J.D. (2008). Columbus and other cannibals: The wétiko disease of exploitation, imperialism, and terrorism, rev ed. New York: Seven Stories Press.

Four Arrows, aka Jacobs, D.T. (Ed.) (2006). Unlearning the language of conquest: Scholars challenge anti-Indianism in America. Austin: University of Texas Press.

Garner, A., Yogman, M., Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health, Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, Council on Early Childhood. (2021). Preventing childhood toxic stress: Partnering with families and communities to promote relational health. Pediatrics148(2), e2021052582

Gleason, T., & Narvaez, D. (2019). Beyond resilience to thriving: Optimizing child wellbeing. International Journal of Wellbeing9(4), 60-79. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5502/ijw.v9i4.987

Gottlieb, G. (2002). On the epigenetic evolution of species-specific perception: The developmental manifold concept. Cognitive Development, 17, 1287–1300.

Greer, A. (Ed.) (2000). The Jesuit relations: Natives and missionaries in seventeenth-century North America. Bedford/St. Martin’s.

Greven, P. (1977). The Protestant temperament: Pattersn of child-rearing, religious experience and the self- in early America. New York: Knopf.

Greven, P. (1991). Spare the child: The religious roots of punishment and the psychological impact of physical abuse. New York: Knopf.

Hewlett, B.S., & Lamb, M.E. (2005). Hunter-gatherer childhoods: evolutionary, developmental and cultural perspectives. New Brunswick, NJ: Aldine.

Jacobs, A. (2001). Original sin: A cultural history. Harper One.

Karr-Morse, R., & Wiley, M.S. (1997). Ghosts from the nursery: Tracing the roots of violence. New York: Atlantic Monthly Press.

Karr-Morse, R., & Wiley, M.S. (2012). Scared sick: The role of childhood trauma in adult disease. New York: Basic Books.

Konner, M. (2005). Hunter-gatherer infancy and childhood: The !Kung and others. In B. Hewlett & M. Lamb (Eds.), Hunter-gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 19-64). New Brunswick, NJ: Aldine Transaction.

Lanius, R. A., Vermetten, E., & Pain, C. (2010). The impact of early life trauma on health and disease: The hidden epidemic. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Lee, S. J., Grogan-Kaylor, A., Berger. L. M. (2014). Parental spanking of 1-year-old children and subsequent child protective services involvement. Child Abuse and Neglect, 38(5), 875-883. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2014.01.018. 

McEwen, B. S. (2017). Neurobiological and systemic effects of chronic stress. Chronic Stress1, 2470547017692328.

McPherson, D.H., & Rabb, J.D. (2011). Indian from the inside: Native American philosophy and cultural renewal, 2nd ed.Jefferson, NC: MacFarland & Co.

Menakem, R. (2017). My grandmother’s hands: Racialized trauma and the pathway to mending our hearts and bodies. Las Vegas: Central Recovery press.

Miller, A. (1990). For your own good: Hidden cruelty in child-rearing and the roots of violence. New York, NY: Noonday Press.

Moloney, J.C. (1949). The magic cloak: A contribution to the psychology of authoritarianism. Wakefield, MA: Montrose Press.

Murgatroyd, C., & Spengler, D. (2011). Epigenetic programming of the HPA axis: early life decides. Stress, 14(6), 581-9. DOI:10.3109/10253890.2011.602146.

Narvaez, D. (2013). The 99%–Development and socialization within an evolutionary context: Growing up to become “A good and useful human being.” In D. Fry (Ed.), War, peace and human nature: The convergence of evolutionary and cultural views (pp. 643-672). New York: Oxford University Press. 

Narvaez, D. (2014). Neurobiology and the development of human morality: Evolution, culture and wisdom. New York: Norton.

Narvaez, D. (2019). Original practices for becoming and being human. In Narvaez, D., Four Arrows, Halton, E., Collier, B., Enderle, G. (Eds.), Indigenous sustainable wisdom: First Nation knowhow for global flourishing (pp. 90-110). New York: Peter Lang.

Narvaez, D., & Tarsha, M. (2021). The missing mind: Contrasting civilization with non-civilization development and functioning. In T. Henley & M. Rossano (Eds.), Psychology and cognitive archaeology: An Interdisciplinary approach to the study of the human mind (pp. 55-69). London: Routledge. 

Narvaez, D. (2021). Species-typical phronesis for a living planet. In M. De Caro & M.S. Vaccarezza (Eds.)Practical Wisdom: Philosophical and Psychological Perspectives (pp. 160-180). London: Routledge.

Ribble, Margaret (1943). The rights of infants. NY: Columbia University Press.

Schore, A.N. (2003). Affect dysregulation & disorders of the self. New York: W.W. Norton.

Scott, G.R. (1968). The history of corporal punishment. Guernsey Press.

Trafzer, C.E., Keller, J.A., & Sisquoc, L. (Eds.) (2006). Boarding school blues: Revisiting Amercian Indian educational experiences. University of Nebraska Press.

Tuck, E., & Yang, K.W. (2012). Decolonization is not a metaphor. Decolonization: Indigeneity, Education & Society, 1(1), 1-40.

Winnicott, D.W. (1987). Babies and their mothers. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Leave A Reply

Your email address will not be published.